|
Showing 1 - 25 of
26 matches in All Departments
This volume is the first comprehensive comparative dictionary to
cover the whole of the Chukotko-Kamchatkan family. The genealogical
status of this family (whether from a common source or due to
convergence) has long been controversial, but its coherence as a
family can now be taken as proven. Its geographical position
between Siberia and northernmost America renders it crucial in any
attempt to relate the languages and peoples of these large
linguistic regions. The dictionary consists of cognate sets
arranged alphabetically according to reconstructed proto-forms and
covers all published lexical sources for the languages concerned
(plus a good deal of unpublished material). The criterion for
setting up Proto-Chukotian sets is the existence of clear cognates
in at least two of the four languages: Chukchi, Koryak, Alutor, and
(now extinct) Kerek, and for Proto-Chukotko-Kamchatkan sets
cognates in at least one of these plus Itelmen. Internal loans
between the two branches of the family are indicated - this is
particularly important in the case of the many loans from Koryak to
modern western Itelmen. Proto-Itelmen sets without clear cognates
in Chukotian are listed separately, without reconstructions. The
data is presented in a reader-friendly format, with each set
divided into separate lines for the individual languages concerned
and with a common orthography for all reliable modern forms (given
as full word stems, not just 'roots'). The introduction contains
information on the distribution of the individual languages and
dialects and all sound correspondences relating them, plus a sketch
of what is known of their (pre)historical background. Inflections
and derivational affixes are treated in separate sections, and
Chukchi and English proto-form indexes allows multiple routes of
access to the data. A full reference list of sources is included.
This is an engaging study of the mental lexicon - the way in which
the form and meaning of words is stored by speakers of specific
languages. Fortescue attempts to narrow the gap between the results
of experimental neurology and the concerns of theoretical
linguistics in the area of lexical semantics. The prime goal as
regards linguistic theory is to show how matters of lexical
organization can be analysed and discussed within a neurologically
informed framework that is both adaptable and constrained. It
combines the perspectives of distributed network modelling and
linguistic semantics, and draws upon the accruing evidence from
neuroimaging studies as regards the cortical regions involved. It
engages with a number of controversial current issues in both
disciplines. This text is intended as a tool for linguists
interested in psychological adequacy and the latest advances in
Cognitive Science. It provides a principled means of distinguishing
those semantic features required by a mental lexicon that have a
direct bearing on grammar from those that do not. "A Neural Network
Model of Lexical Organisation" is essential reading for researchers
in neurolinguistics and lexical semantics. "Continuum Studies in
Theoretical Linguistics" publishes work at the forefront of
present-day developments in the field. The series is open to
studies from all branches of theoretical linguistics and to the
full range of theoretical frameworks. Titles in the series present
original research that makes a new and significant contribution and
are aimed primarily at scholars in the field, but are clear and
accessible, making them useful also to students, to new researchers
and to scholars in related disciplines.
In building up a scenario for the arrival on the shores of Alaska
of speakers of languages related to Eskimo-Aleut with genetic roots
deep within Sineria, this book touches upon a number of issues in
contemporary historical linguistics and archaeology. The Arctic
"gateway" to the New World, by acting as a bottleneck, has allowed
only small groups of mobile hunter-gatherers through during
specific propitious periods, and thus provides a unique testing
ground for theories about population and language movements in
pre-agricultural times. Owing to the historically attested
prevalence of language shifts and other contact phenomena in the
region, it is arguable that the spread of genes and the spread of
language have been out of step since the earliest reconstructable
times, contrary to certain views of their linkage. Proposals that
have been put forward in the past concerning the affiliations of
Eskimo-Aleut languages are followed up in the light of recent
progress in reconstructing the proto-languages concerned. Those
linking Eskimo-Aleut with the Uralic languages and Yukagir are
particularly promising, and reconstructions for many common
elements are presented. The entire region "Great Beringia" is
scoured for typological evidence in the form of anomalies and
constellations of uncommon traits diagnostic of affiliation or
contact. The various threads lead back to mesolithic times in south
central Siberia, when speakers of a "Uralo-Siberian" mesh of
related languages appears to have moved along the major waterways
of Siberia. Such a scenario would acount for the present
distribution of these languages and the results of their meeting
with remnants of earlier linguistic waves from the Old World to the
New.
This book is intended as counter-evidence to the perception that
Linguistics is a domain of dusty schoolroom grammar. It follows
that linguistics can be characterised differently than as
proponents of theoretical orientations who spend their brief breaks
from their bone-dry work bashing each other over the head with
their different favourite abstractions. The discipline may appear
to outsiders as fragmented and -- worse still -- lacking in
relevance to the real world outside its gates. This book
demonstrates that Linguistics, in all its varied branches, can be
entertaining as well as thought-provoking, and that its domain is
indeed a coherent one despite all the internecine squabbling. In an
unconventional way Michael Fortescue introduces his subject as a
kind of fable with a historical moral that professional linguists,
as well as students, should enjoy as a useful commentary on the
state of the discipline today.
This handbook offers an extensive crosslinguistic and
cross-theoretical survey of polysynthetic languages, in which
single multi-morpheme verb forms can express what would be whole
sentences in English. These languages and the problems they raise
for linguistic analyses have long featured prominently in language
descriptions, and yet the essence of polysynthesis remains under
discussion, right down to whether it delineates a distinct,
coherent type, rather than an assortment of frequently co-occurring
traits. Chapters in the first part of the handbook relate
polysynthesis to other issues central to linguistics, such as
complexity, the definition of the word, the nature of the lexicon,
idiomaticity, and to typological features such as argument
structure and head marking. Part two contains areal studies of
those geographical regions of the world where polysynthesis is
particularly common, such as the Arctic and Sub-Arctic and northern
Australia. The third part examines diachronic topics such as
language contact and language obsolence, while part four looks at
acquisition issues in different polysynthetic languages. Finally,
part five contains detailed grammatical descriptions of over twenty
languages which have been characterized as polysynthetic, with
special attention given to the presence or absence of potentially
criterial features.
These curious tales all have something to do with language: Does
one really have to queue to be assigned a language at birth? How
could one go about communicating with an alien but sentient
'thing'? Are words sufficient to navigate through the numerical
labyrinth of Manhattan? Can the sum total of all human knowledge be
transferred to electromagnetic circuits in rock? Can one improve
one's memory by acquiring a conservatory in one's autumn years? Can
language be acquired by a vegetal implant? The stories comprising
this volume do not answer any of these questions, but considering
them at least provides food for thought.
Words are escaping from the flagship project of the prestigious
House of Words. Something must be done fast to get them back before
a rival company is brought in to take over the project. But the
lexicographers below deck are not being cooperative. Treated as
slaves they use their time when not supervised to play word games,
facing the threat of the overseer's whip if discovered. The
management suspects that they may be aiding the leakage and their
ringleader is clapped in irons. Emissaries are sent to negotiate
with the words now living in riotous anarchy in the wild woods, but
with little success. The words have certain demands if they are to
return. An ultimatum is relayed to the irascible editor-in-chief,
who cannot allow this undermining of editorial policy. His aging
boss, the publisher, who is himself having difficulty remembering
words, is only interested in cost-efficiency. A solution of a
surprising kind is finally brokered by a humble female servitor
sympathetic to the lexicographers? cause.
James, an IT consultant engaged in upgrading the computer system of
the Brain Imaging Institute at Cambridge, discovers something
strange in the institute's server - a number of encrypted titles of
books. But before he can investigate further the institute's
director is dismissed for appropriating institute funds to acquire
a large quantity of beryllium fluoride from Russia for his own
obscure ends. He disappears. Since James' company is engaged in
nanotechnological research involving this substance he is more than
a little curious to know what the director has been up to. A visit
to the local library in order to track down one of the encrypted
titles - "Christina's Book" - leads to an initial dead-end: no such
book exists, although the title appears as an otherwise blank
catalogue entry under the director's name. What does it refer to?
James will finally discover - but only after a massive solar storm
has undermined the intended results of an extraordinary clandestine
experiment...
Each of the prose poems that constitute the first part of this
volume juxtaposes at least two widely different images and ways of
reading them. The result - an either/or choice between
incompatibles - can be surprising. Those of the second part are
based on more free-wheeling association and word play. All good
fun.
This tightly constructed novel is set in the fictitious village of
Slatefield, where dinosaur bones were first found in England. The
arrival of a writer of popular books at the house of his brother,
an Oxford archaeologist, stirs up an old rivalry. A strange stone
has been unearthed from a local quarry bearing what looks like an
ancient script and the two brothers have very different
interpretations of its contents. For the archaeologist it is
clearly a fake, but for his brother and his artist wife it opens a
window to a prehistoric civilization, going back even to the age of
the giant dragonflies. There is also a land dispute over the site
of a Roman villa, and disagreement among the villagers over the
possible reopening of the slate mines. A humorous element is
introduced by the bumbling new vicar who tries to smooth over all
these disputes with words of unorthodox Whiteheadian wisdom.
Science and imagination are at loggerheads, and their resolution
(of sorts) provides a surprising ending.
A grumpy old hypochondriac is led into a world of dream and memory
by certain pieces he finds separated out from the puzzle he is
putting together. They are the key to the walled town represented
in the surface of the puzzle - and to a secret locked within it.
The book itself constitutes a puzzle: the 'solution' is a fully
interlocking picture of the tragi-comic narrator, as he tries to
make head or tail of his aimless life. Amongst the ambiguous
denizens of the puzzle world he glimpses a girl he once loved and
lost...
The walled - and very private - village of Greendale is not all
that it appears to be from the outside. Superficially it embodies
all that is quaint and desirable in the English country way of
life. Within it is concealed an exclusive language academy catering
for wealthy foreigners. This curious institution purports to teach
'perfect' English - with a 100 % success rate - and successful
graduation holds out the promise of integration into the English
upper crust. Everything seems light and amusing at the onset, and
the latest electronic technology is regarded just as fun by the new
intake of students. But gradually the gadgetry grows more and more
intrusive into their private lives and it emerges that both the
teaching staff and the 'villagers' are more eccentric than either
the students or the naive new teacher Adrian have bargained for.
Trying to enter the village under false pretences can lead to
unpleasant consequences, but when students try to escape things can
grow nasty indeed...
This unconventional introduction to the linguistic discipline of
semantics - and pragmatics - takes the form of a series of zany
dialogues, each illustrating a particular topic. They do this by
breaking the rules that govern language usage in such a way as to
bring home their hidden existence with a jolt. The intention is to
render the significance of these abstractions more tangible and to
sharpen the reader's awareness of what lurks beneath the surface of
more 'normal' human communication. The notion of context is crucial
throughout: it is the key to understanding the richer meaning of
both individual words and whole utterances. Following each dialogue
there are some definitions and a brief discussion of the topics
concerned, together with references for more serious reading. The
collection arose from the author's experience as professor of
linguistics at the University of Copenhagen, in particular with the
functional and cognitive aspects of language.
Annie is about to start her last year at boarding school in
Abingdon when Uncle George, whom she barely remembers, shows up out
of the blue. He is drunk and starts off on an elaborate
confabulation about an event he has been witness to on Abingdon
Bridge. His appearance in Annie's life will turn her life upside
down and provide her with a seemingly endless source of
entertainment. He has returned to his old stomping ground after a
mysterious spell 'abroad'. He evidently has an intimate knowledge
of all aspects of the town's life, its history and local characters
and, above all, a thorough knowledge of the town's pubs. Annie,
embarrassed by his behaviour in front of others, keeps her meetings
with him secret. Uncle George is chronically incapable of sticking
to the truth, but one of the few definite things one learns about
his past is that his connection to Annie's family is more
complicated than she suspected. But who exactly is this
disreputable, loveable, but ultimately pathetic character...?
This is an engaging study of the mental lexicon: the way in which
the form and meaning of words is stored by speakers of specific
languages. Fortescue attempts to narrow the gap between the results
of experimental neurology and the concerns of theoretical
linguistics in the area of lexical semantics. The prime goal as
regards linguistic theory is to show how matters of lexical
organization can be analysed and discussed within a neurologically
informed framework that is both adaptable and constrained. It
combines the perspectives of distributed network modelling and
linguistic semantics, and draws upon the accruing evidence from
neuroimaging studies as regards the cortical regions involved. It
engages with a number of controversial current issues in both
disciplines. This text is intended as a tool for linguists
interested in psychological adequacy and the latest advances in
Cognitive Science. It provides a principled means of distinguishing
those semantic features required by a mental lexicon that have a
direct bearing on grammar from those that do not. A Neural Network
Model of Lexical Organisation is essential reading for researchers
in neurolinguistics and lexical semantics.
This is an extension of the author's earlier volume, "Eskimo
Orientation Systems". This time it covers all the contiguous
languages -- and cultures -- across the northern Pacific rim from
Van-couver Island in Canada to Hokkaido in northern Japan, plus the
adjacent Arctic coasts of Alaska and Chukotka. These form a testing
ground for recent theories con-cerning the nature and
classification of orientation systems and their shared 'frames of
reference', in particular the many varieties of 'landmark' systems
typifying the Arctic and sub-Arctic. Despite the wide variety of
languages spoken here (all of them endangered), there is much in
common as regards their overlapping geographical settings and the
ways in which terms for orientation within the microcosm (the
house) and within the macrocosm (the surrounding environment) mesh
throughout the region. This is illustrated with numerous maps and
diagrams, from both coastal and inland sites. Attention is paid to
ambiguities and anomalies within the systems revealed by the data,
as these may be clues to pre-historic movements of the populations
con-cerned -- from a riverine setting to the coast, from the coast
to inland, or more complex successive displacements. Cultural
factors over and beyond environmental determinism are discussed
within this broad context.
|
You may like...
Higher
Michael Buble
CD
(1)
R459
Discovery Miles 4 590
Gloria
Sam Smith
CD
R187
R177
Discovery Miles 1 770
|