|
Showing 1 - 4 of
4 matches in All Departments
Competence and incompetence are constructs that emerge in the
social milieu of everyday life. Individuals are continually making
and revising judgments about each other's abilities as they
interact. The flexible, situated view of competence conveyed by the
research of the authors in this volume is a departure from the way
that competence is usually thought about in the fields of
communication disabilities and education. In the social
constructivist view, competence is not a fixed mass, residing
within an individual, or a fixed judgment, defined externally.
Rather, it is variable, sensitive to what is going on in the here
and now, and coconstructed by those present. Constructions of
competence are tied to evaluations implicit in the communication of
the participants as well as to explicit evaluations of how things
are going.
The authors address the social construction of competence in a
variety of situations: engaging in therapy for communication and
other disorders, working and living with people with disabilities,
speaking a second language, living with deafness, and giving and
receiving instruction. Their studies focus on adults and children,
including those with disabilities (aphasia, traumatic brain injury,
augmentative systems users), as they go about managing their lives
and identities. They examine the all-important context in which
participants make competence judgments, assess the impact of
implicit judgments and formal diagnoses, and look at the types of
evaluations made during interaction.
This book makes an argument all helping professionals need to
hear: institutional, clinical, and social practices promoting
judgments must be changed to practices that are more positive and
empowering.
Competence and incompetence are constructs that emerge in the
social milieu of everyday life. Individuals are continually making
and revising judgments about each other's abilities as they
interact. The flexible, situated view of competence conveyed by the
research of the authors in this volume is a departure from the way
that competence is usually thought about in the fields of
communication disabilities and education. In the social
constructivist view, competence is not a fixed mass, residing
within an individual, or a fixed judgment, defined externally.
Rather, it is variable, sensitive to what is going on in the here
and now, and coconstructed by those present. Constructions of
competence are tied to evaluations implicit in the communication of
the participants as well as to explicit evaluations of how things
are going.
The authors address the social construction of competence in a
variety of situations: engaging in therapy for communication and
other disorders, working and living with people with disabilities,
speaking a second language, living with deafness, and giving and
receiving instruction. Their studies focus on adults and children,
including those with disabilities (aphasia, traumatic brain injury,
augmentative systems users), as they go about managing their lives
and identities. They examine the all-important context in which
participants make competence judgments, assess the impact of
implicit judgments and formal diagnoses, and look at the types of
evaluations made during interaction.
This book makes an argument all helping professionals need to
hear: institutional, clinical, and social practices promoting
judgments must be changed to practices that are more positive and
empowering.
This volume describes the theoretical and empirical results of a
seven year collaborative effort of cognitive scientists to develop
a computational model for narrative understanding. Disciplines
represented include artificial intelligence, cognitive psychology,
communicative disorders, education, English, geography,
linguistics, and philosophy. The book argues for an organized
representational system -- a Deictic Center (DC) -- which is
constructed by readers from language in a text combined with their
world knowledge.
As readers approach a new text they need to gather and maintain
information about who the participants are and where and when the
events take place. This information plays a central role in
understanding the narrative. The editors claim that readers
maintain this information without explicit textual reminders by
including it in their mental model of the story world.
Because of the centrality of the temporal, spatial, and character
information in narratives, they developed their notion of a DC as a
crucial part of the reader's mental model of the narrative. The
events that carry the temporal and spatial core of the narrative
are linguistically and conceptually constrained according to
certain principles that can be relatively well defined. A narrative
obviously unfolds one word, or one sentence, at a time. This volume
suggests that cognitively a narrative usually unfolds one place and
time at a time. This spatio-temporal location functions as part of
the DC of the narrative. It is the "here" and "now" of the reader's
"mind's eye" in the world of the story.
Organized into seven parts, this book describes the goal of the
cognitive science project resulting in this volume, the
methodological approaches taken, and the history of the
collaborative effort. It provides a historical and theoretical
background underlying the DC theory, including discussions of
deixis in language and the nature of fiction. It goes on to outline
the computational framework and how it is used to represent deixis
in narrative, and details the linguistic devices implicated in the
DC theory. Other subjects covered include: crosslinguistic
indicators of subjectivity, psychological investigations of the use
of deixis by children and adults as they process narratives,
conversation, direction giving, implications for emerging literacy,
and a narrator's experience in writing a short story.
This volume describes the theoretical and empirical results of a
seven year collaborative effort of cognitive scientists to develop
a computational model for narrative understanding. Disciplines
represented include artificial intelligence, cognitive psychology,
communicative disorders, education, English, geography,
linguistics, and philosophy. The book argues for an organized
representational system -- a Deictic Center (DC) -- which is
constructed by readers from language in a text combined with their
world knowledge.
As readers approach a new text they need to gather and maintain
information about who the participants are and where and when the
events take place. This information plays a central role in
understanding the narrative. The editors claim that readers
maintain this information without explicit textual reminders by
including it in their mental model of the story world.
Because of the centrality of the temporal, spatial, and character
information in narratives, they developed their notion of a DC as a
crucial part of the reader's mental model of the narrative. The
events that carry the temporal and spatial core of the narrative
are linguistically and conceptually constrained according to
certain principles that can be relatively well defined. A narrative
obviously unfolds one word, or one sentence, at a time. This volume
suggests that cognitively a narrative usually unfolds one place and
time at a time. This spatio-temporal location functions as part of
the DC of the narrative. It is the "here" and "now" of the reader's
"mind's eye" in the world of the story.
Organized into seven parts, this book describes the goal of the
cognitive science project resulting in this volume, the
methodological approaches taken, and the history of the
collaborative effort. It provides a historical and theoretical
background underlying the DC theory, including discussions of
deixis in language and the nature of fiction. It goes on to outline
the computational framework and how it is used to represent deixis
in narrative, and details the linguistic devices implicated in the
DC theory. Other subjects covered include: crosslinguistic
indicators of subjectivity, psychological investigations of the use
of deixis by children and adults as they process narratives,
conversation, direction giving, implications for emerging literacy,
and a narrator's experience in writing a short story.
|
|