|
Showing 1 - 5 of
5 matches in All Departments
As Nixon's unpopularity increased during Watergate, his nose and
jowls grew to impossible proportions in published caricatures. Yet
the caricatures remained instantly recognizable. Caricatures can
even be superportraits, with the paradoxical quality of being more
like the face than the face itself. How can we recognize such
distorted images? Do caricatures derive their power from some
special property of a face recognition system or from some more
general property of recognition systems? What kind of mental
representations and recognition processes make caricatures so
effective? What can the power of caricatures tell us about
recognition? In seeking to answer these questions, the author
assembles clues from a variety of sources: the invention and
development of caricatures by artists, the exploitation of extreme
signals in animal communication systems, and studies of how humans,
other animals and connectionist recognition systems respond to
caricatures. Several conclusions emerge. The power of caricatures
is ubiquitous. Caricatures can be superportraits for humans, other
animals and computer recognition systems. They are effective for a
variety of stimuli, not just faces. They are effective whether
objects are mentally represented as deviations from a norm or
average member of the class, or as absolute feature values on a set
of dimensions. Exaggeration of crucial norm-deviation features,
distinctiveness, and resemblance to caricatured memory traces are
all potential sources of the power of caricature. Superportraits
will be of interest to students of cognitive psychology,
perception, the visual arts and animal behavior.
Contents: Introduction. The Nature of Caricature. Caricatures by Computer. Peacock's Tails and other Natural Caricatures. The Power of Extremes. The Psychology of Caricatures. Caricatures and Face Recognition. The View from Here.
From a barrage of photons, we readily and effortlessly recognize
the faces of our friends, and the familiar objects and scenes
around us. However, these tasks cannot be simple for our visual
systems--faces are all extremely similar as visual patterns, and
objects look quite different when viewed from different viewpoints.
How do our visual systems solve these problems? The contributors to
this volume seek to answer this question by exploring how analytic
and holistic processes contribute to our perception of faces,
objects, and scenes. The role of parts and wholes in perception has
been studied for a century, beginning with the debate between
Structuralists, who championed the role of elements, and Gestalt
psychologists, who argued that the whole was different from the sum
of its parts. This is the first volume to focus on the current
state of the debate on parts versus wholes as it exists in the
field of visual perception by bringing together the views of the
leading researchers. Too frequently, researchers work in only one
domain, so they are unaware of the ways in which holistic and
analytic processing are defined in different areas. The
contributors to this volume ask what analytic and holistic
processes are like; whether they contribute differently to the
perception of faces, objects, and scenes; whether different
cognitive and neural mechanisms code holistic and analytic
information; whether a single, universal system can be sufficient
for visual-information processing, and whether our subjective
experience of holistic perception might be nothing more than a
compelling illusion. The result is a snapshot of the current
thinking on how the processing of wholes and parts contributesto
our remarkable ability to recognize faces, objects, and scenes, and
an illustration of the diverse conceptions of analytic and holistic
processing that currently coexist, and the variety of approaches
that have been brought to bear on the issues.
Adaptation phenomena provide striking examples of perceptual
plasticity and offer valuable insight into the mechanisms of visual
coding. The technique of psychophysical adaptation has aptly been
termed the psychologist's microelectrode because of its usefulness
in investigating the coding of sensory information in the human
brain. Its broader relevance though is illustrated by the
increasing use of adaptation to study more cognitive aspects of
vision such as the mechanisms of face perception and the neural
substrates of visual awareness. This book brings together a
collection of studies from international researchers, which
demonstrate the brain's remarkable capacity to adapt its
representation of the visual world in response to changes in its
environment. A major theme throughout is that adaptation at all
stages of visual processing serves a functional role in the
efficient representation of the prevailing visual environment.
Information about the visual world is coded in the rate at which
neurons fire. However, neurons can only respond over a certain
range of firing rates. Adaptation of the way in which neurons code
visual information tends to make optimal use of this limited
response range. Though these principles are well established at the
level of light adaptation in the retina, it is only relatively
recently that researchers have started to look for analogous
behaviour at the higher levels of the visual system. This book is
the first to bring together evidence that adaptation in high-level
vision, as at the lower levels, serves to fit the mind to the
world.
|
You may like...
Hoe Ek Dit Onthou
Francois Van Coke, Annie Klopper
Paperback
R300
R219
Discovery Miles 2 190
The Northman
Alexander Skarsgard, Nicole Kidman, …
Blu-ray disc
(1)
R210
Discovery Miles 2 100
Loot
Nadine Gordimer
Paperback
(2)
R383
R310
Discovery Miles 3 100
Loot
Nadine Gordimer
Paperback
(2)
R383
R310
Discovery Miles 3 100
|