|
Showing 1 - 8 of
8 matches in All Departments
'A landmark in social thought. Henrich may go down as the most
influential social scientist of the first half of the twenty-first
century' MATTHEW SYED Do you identify yourself by your profession
or achievements, rather than your family network? Do you cultivate
your unique attributes and goals? If so, perhaps you are WEIRD:
raised in a society that is Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich
and Democratic. Unlike most who have ever lived, WEIRD people are
highly individualistic, nonconformist, analytical and
control-oriented. How did WEIRD populations become so
psychologically peculiar? What part did these differences play in
our history, and what do they mean for our collective identity?
Joseph Henrich, who developed the game-changing concept of WEIRD,
uses leading-edge research in anthropology, psychology, economics
and evolutionary biology to explore how changing family structures,
marriage practices and religious beliefs in the Middle Ages shaped
the Western mind, laying the foundations for the world we know
today. Brilliant, provocative, engaging and surprising, this
landmark study will revolutionize your understanding of who - and
how - we are now. 'Phenomenal ... The only theory I am aware of
that attempts to explain broad patterns of human psychology on a
global scale' Washington Post 'You will never look again in the
same way at your own seemingly universal values' Uta Frith,
Professor of Cognitive Development, University College London
Humans are a puzzling species. On the one hand, we struggle to
survive on our own in the wild, often failing to overcome even
basic challenges, like obtaining food, building shelters, or
avoiding predators. On the other hand, human groups have produced
ingenious technologies, sophisticated languages, and complex
institutions that have permitted us to successfully expand into a
vast range of diverse environments. What has enabled us to dominate
the globe, more than any other species, while remaining virtually
helpless as lone individuals? This book shows that the secret of
our success lies not in our innate intelligence, but in our
collective brains--on the ability of human groups to socially
interconnect and learn from one another over generations. Drawing
insights from lost European explorers, clever chimpanzees, mobile
hunter-gatherers, neuroscientific findings, ancient bones, and the
human genome, Joseph Henrich demonstrates how our collective brains
have propelled our species' genetic evolution and shaped our
biology. Our early capacities for learning from others produced
many cultural innovations, such as fire, cooking, water containers,
plant knowledge, and projectile weapons, which in turn drove the
expansion of our brains and altered our physiology, anatomy, and
psychology in crucial ways. Later on, some collective brains
generated and recombined powerful concepts, such as the lever,
wheel, screw, and writing, while also creating the institutions
that continue to alter our motivations and perceptions. Henrich
shows how our genetics and biology are inextricably interwoven with
cultural evolution, and how culture-gene interactions launched our
species on an extraordinary evolutionary trajectory. Tracking clues
from our ancient past to the present, The Secret of Our Success
explores how the evolution of both our cultural and social natures
produce a collective intelligence that explains both our species'
immense success and the origins of human uniqueness.
What motives underlie the ways humans interact socially? Are these
the same for all societies? Are these part of our nature, or
influenced by our environments? Over the last decade, research in
experimental economics has emphatically falsified the textbook
representation of Homo economicus. Hundreds of experiments suggest
that people care not only about their own material payoffs, but
also about such things as fairness, equity, and reciprocity.
However, this research left fundamental questions unanswered: Are
such social preferences stable components of human nature, or are
they modulated by economic, social, and cultural environments?
Until now, experimental research could not address this question
because virtually all subjects had been university students.
Combining ethnographic and experimental approaches to fill this
gap, this book breaks new ground in reporting the results of a
large cross-cultural study aimed at determining the sources of
social (non-selfish) preferences that underlie the diversity of
human sociality. In this study, the same experiments carried out
with university students were performed in fifteen small-scale
societies exhibiting a wide variety of social, economic, and
cultural conditions. The results show that the variation in
behaviour is far greater than previously thought, and that the
differences between societies in market integration and the
importance of cooperation explain a substantial portion of this
variation, which individual-level economic and demographic
variables could not. The results also trace the extent to which
experimental play mirrors patterns of interaction found in everyday
life. The book includes a succinct but substantive introduction to
the use of game theory as an analytical tool, and to its use in the
social sciences for the rigorous testing of hypotheses about
fundamental aspects of social behaviour outside artificially
constructed laboratories. The editors also summarize the results of
the fifteen case studies in a suggestive chapter about the scope of
the project.
Cooperation among humans is one of the keys to our great
evolutionary success. Natalie and Joseph Henrich examine this
phenomena with a unique fusion of theoretical work on the evolution
of cooperation, ethnographic descriptions of social behavior, and a
range of other experimental results. Their experimental and
ethnographic data come from a small, insular group of middle-class
Iraqi Christians called Chaldeans, living in metro Detroit, whom
the Henrichs use as an example to show how kinship relations,
ethnicity, and culturally transmitted traditions provide the key to
explaining the evolution of cooperation over multiple generations.
What motives underlie the ways humans interact socially? Are these
the same for all societies? Are these part of our nature, or
influenced by our environments? Over the last decade, research in
experimental economics has emphatically falsified the textbook
representation of Homo economicus. Literally hundreds of
experiments suggest that people care not only about their own
material payoffs, but also about such things as fairness, equity
and reciprocity. However, this research left fundamental questions
unanswered: Are such social preferences stable components of human
nature; or, are they modulated by economic, social and cultural
environments? Until now, experimental research could not address
this question because virtually all subjects had been university
students, and while there are cultural differences among student
populations throughout the world, these differences are small
compared to the full range of human social and cultural
environments. A vast amount of ethnographic and historical research
suggests that people's motives are influenced by economic, social,
and cultural environments, yet such methods can only yield
circumstantial evidence about human motives. Combining ethnographic
and experimental approaches to fill this gap, this book breaks new
ground in reporting the results of a large cross-cultural study
aimed at determining the sources of social (non-selfish)
preferences that underlie the diversity of human sociality. The
same experiments which provided evidence for social preferences
among university students were performed in fifteen small-scale
societies exhibiting a wide variety of social, economic and
cultural conditions by experienced field researchers who had also
done long-term ethnographic field work in these societies. The
findings of these experiments demonstrated that no society in which
experimental behaviour is consistent with the canonical model of
self-interest. Indeed, results showed that the variation in
behaviour is far greater than previously thought, and that the
differences between societies in market integration and the
importance of cooperation explain a substantial portion of this
variation, which individual-level economic and demographic
variables could not. Finally, the extent to which experimental play
mirrors patterns of interaction found in everyday life is traced.
The book starts with a succinct but substantive introduction to the
use of game theory as an analytical tool and its use in the social
sciences for the rigorous testing of hypotheses about fundamental
aspects of social behaviour outside artificially constructed
laboratories. The results of the fifteen case studies are
summarized in a suggestive chapter about the scope of the project.
Cooperation among humans is one of the keys to our great
evolutionary success. Natalie and Joseph Henrich examine this
phenomena with a unique fusion of theoretical work on the evolution
of cooperation, ethnographic descriptions of social behavior, and a
range of other experimental results.< br> Their experimental
and ethnographic data come from a small, insular group of
middle-class Iraqi Christians called Chaldeans, living in metro
Detroit, whom the Henrichs use as an example to show how kinship
relations, ethnicity, and culturally transmitted traditions provide
the key to explaining the< br> evolution of cooperation over
multiple generations.
German description: Unser moralisches Empfinden ist von den
Emotionen Gluck und Angst gepragt. Sie gelten gar als Motor
moralischen Handelns. Doch in unserer Gesellschaft herrscht nach
wie vor eine strikte Trennung von Eigeninteresse und Moral. Wahrend
Freiheit und Autonomie gepriesen werden, sollen wir in erster Linie
Gefuhle vertreten, die nicht unsere eigenen sind. Das vergrossert
die Kluft zwischen Sein und Schein. Experimentelle Forschung im
Grenzbereich von Moralpsychologie, Neurowissenschaften und
Verhaltensokonomie wie auch neuere Erkenntnisse aus den
interdisziplinaren Geistes- und insbesondere
Religionswissenschaften lassen ein neues Bild des Menschen
entstehen. Es hat wenig mit dem eines rationalen und an
Idealvorstellungen orientierten Entscheidungsfinders zu tun, wie es
bisher in Okonomie und Ethik dominiert hat. Demnach gibt es weder
den Menschen, der ausschliesslich an kurzfristiger und rein
materieller Nutzenmaximierung interessiert ist, noch gibt es den
komplett uneigennutzigen Typus, der immer nur an das Wohl der
Allgemeinheit denkt. Mit einem Interview mit dem israelischen
Historiker Shlomo Sand (Die Erfindung des judischen Volkes).
|
You may like...
Loot
Nadine Gordimer
Paperback
(2)
R383
R310
Discovery Miles 3 100
Workplace law
John Grogan
Paperback
R900
R820
Discovery Miles 8 200
|