|
Books > Social sciences > Psychology > Cognition & cognitive psychology
Rethinking Thought takes readers into the minds of 30 creative
thinkers to show how greatly the experience of thought can vary. It
is dedicated to anyone who has ever been told, "You're not
thinking!", because his or her way of thinking differs so much from
a spouse's, employer's, or teacher's. The book focuses on
individual experiences with visual mental images and verbal
language that are used in planning, problem-solving, reflecting,
remembering, and forging new ideas. It approaches the question of
what thinking is by analyzing variations in the way thinking feels.
Written by neuroscientist-turned-literary scholar Laura Otis,
Rethinking Thought juxtaposes creative thinkers' insights with
recent neuroscientific discoveries about visual mental imagery,
verbal language, and thought. Presenting the results of new,
interview-based research, it offers verbal portraits of novelist
Salman Rushdie, engineer Temple Grandin, American Poet Laureate
Natasha Trethewey, and Nobel prize-winning biologist Elizabeth
Blackburn. It also depicts the unique mental worlds of two
award-winning painters, a flamenco dancer, a game designer, a
cartoonist, a lawyer-novelist, a theoretical physicist, and a
creator of multi-agent software. Treating scientists and artists
with equal respect, it creates a dialogue in which neuroscientific
findings and the introspections of creative thinkers engage each
other as equal partners. The interviews presented in this book
indicate that many creative people enter fields requiring skills
that don't come naturally. Instead, they choose professions that
demand the hardest work and the greatest mental growth. Instead of
classifying people as "visual" or "verbal," educators and managers
need to consider how thinkers combine visual and verbal skills and
how those abilities can be further developed. By showing how
greatly individual experiences of thought can vary, this book aims
to help readers in all professions better understand and respect
the diverse people with whom they work.
Multitasking is all around us: the office worker interrupted by a
phone call, the teenager texting while driving, the salesperson
chatting while entering an order. When multitasking, the mind
juggles all the many tasks we're doing this second, this hour, this
week, and tries to perform them together-sometimes with great ease,
sometimes with great difficulty. We don't often stop to think about
how exactly we accomplish these feats of multitasking great and
small. How do we switch from one task to another? What types of
multitasking are disruptive, and when are they most disruptive? And
ultimately, how can we take advantage of the benefits of
multitasking while alleviating its negative effects in our daily
lives?
This book presents the theory of threaded cognition, a theory that
aims to explain the multitasking mind. The theory states that
multitasking behavior can be expressed as cognitive
threads-independent streams of thought that weave through the
mind's processing resources to produce multitasking behavior, and
sometimes experience conflicts to produce multitasking
interference. Grounded in the ACT-R cognitive architecture,
threaded cognition incorporates computational representations and
mechanisms used to simulate and predict multitasking behavior and
performance.
The book describes the implications of threaded cognition theory
across three traditionally disparate domains: concurrent
multitasking (doing multiple tasks at once), sequential
multitasking (interrupting and resuming tasks), and multitask skill
acquisition (learning and practicing multiple tasks). The work
stresses the importance of unifying basic and applied research by
alternating between in-depth descriptions of basic research
phenomena and broader treatments of phenomena in applied domains,
such as driver distraction and human-computer interaction. The book
also includes practical guidelines for designers of interactive
systems intended for multitasking contexts.
Consciousness is a perennial source of mystification in the
philosophy of mind: how can processes in the brain amount to
conscious experiences? Robert Kirk uses the notion of `raw feeling'
to bridge the intelligibility gap between our knowledge of
ourselves as physical organisms and our knowledge of ourselves as
subjects of experience; he argues that there is no need for
recourse to dualism or private mental objects. The task is to
understand how the truth about raw feeling could be strictly
implied by narrowly physical truths. Kirk's explanation turns on an
account of what it is to be a subject of conscious perceptual
experience. He offers penetrating analyses of the problems of
consciousness and suggests novel solutions which, unlike their
rivals, can be accepted without gritting one's teeth. His sustained
defence of non-reductive physicalism shows that we need not abandon
hope of finding a solution to the mind-body problem.
Simple Heuristics in a Social World invites readers to discover the
simple heuristics that people use to navigate the complexities and
surprises of environments populated with others. The social world
is a terrain where humans and other animals compete with
conspecifics for myriad resources, including food, mates, and
status, and where rivals grant the decision maker little time for
deep thought, protracted information search, or complex
calculations. Yet, the social world also encompasses domains where
social animals such as humans can learn from one another and can
forge alliances with one another to boost their chances of success.
According to the book's thesis, the undeniable complexity of the
social world does not dictate cognitive complexity as many scholars
of rationality argue. Rather, it entails circumstances that render
optimization impossible or computationally arduous: intractability,
the existence of incommensurable considerations, and competing
goals. With optimization beyond reach, less can be more. That is,
heuristics--simple strategies for making decisions when time is
pressing and careful deliberation an unaffordable luxury--become
indispensible mental tools. As accurate as or even more accurate
than complex methods when used in the appropriate social
environments, these heuristics are good descriptive models of how
people make many decisions and inferences, but their impressive
performance also poses a normative challenge for optimization
models. In short, the Homo socialis may prove to be a Homo
heuristicus whose intelligence reflects ecological rather than
logical rationality.
This is the last major work of Eugene N. Sokolov, Professor of
Psychophysiology at Moscow State University from 1950 to 2008. It
summarizes the contributions of a lifetime on the neural mechanism
of consciousness. Working at the intersection of psychology,
neurophysiology and mathematics, Sokolov early introduced the
concept of quantifiable 'difference in neuronal activity' and
'cognitive distance' as corresponding metrics in the physical and
mental models of reality. He demonstrated the power of
multidimensional vector mathematics to represent the neural
computations that mediate between the brain's neural model and the
mind's mental model of reality.
Sokolov and colleagues showed a mathematical commonality among the
neuronal mechanisms that mediate the perception of basic features
of visual stimuli including color, brightness, line orientation and
motion. This led to a general vector model linking perceptual and
memory processes to adaptive motor mechanisms. They extended the
model to encompass broader, more complex functions, such as the
perception of emotions in facial expressions, semantic differences
in verbal stimuli and differential executive control mechanisms.
Integrating evidence from human psychophysics, animal
neurophysiology and vector mathematics they developed a unified
model to characterize quantitatively many complex relations between
objective and subjective aspects of reality.
Sokolov's studies of neuronal mechanisms of mental phenomena led
him to distinguish two categories of neurons: 'consciousness
neurons' directly associated with awareness of perceptual,
emotional and cognitive events, and neurons that are necessary for,
but not directly involved in, conscious processes. The book
integrates his findings with major themes shaping twenty-first
century understanding of the brain-mind relationship. It relates
the findings both to work of other Russian investigators, such as
Pavlov, Luria, and Rusinov, and to work of many Western
researchers, including von Bekesy, Eccles, Edelman, Ehrenstein,
Grossberg, John, Koch and Crick, Ledoux, Llinas, Milner, Penfield,
Penrose, Posner, and Schrodinger."
Few conversational topics can be as significant as our troubles in
life, whether everyday and commonplace, or more exceptional and
disturbing. In groundbreaking research conducted with John Lee at
the University of Manchester UK, Gail Jefferson turned the
microscope on how people talk about their troubles, not in any
professional or therapeutic setting, but in their ordinary
conversations with family and friends. Through recordings of
interactions in which people talk about problems they're having
with their children, concerns about their health, financial
problems, marital and relationship difficulties (their own or other
people's), examination failures, dramatic events such as burglaries
or a house fire and other such troubles, Jefferson explores the
interactional dynamics and complexities of introducing such topics,
of how speakers sustain and elaborate their descriptions and
accounts of their troubles, how participants align and affiliate
with one another, and finally manage to move away from such topics.
The studies Jefferson published out of that remarkable period of
research have been collected together in this volume. They are as
insightful and informative about how we talk about our troubles, as
they are innovative in the development and application of
Conversation Analysis. Gail Jefferson (1938-2008) was one of the
co-founders of Conversation Analysis (CA); through her early
collaboration with Harvey Sacks and in her subsequent research, she
laid the foundations for what has become an immensely important
interdisciplinary paradigm. She co-authored, with Harvey Sacks and
Emanuel Schegloff, two of the most highly cited articles ever
published in Language, on turn-taking and repair. These papers were
foundational, as was the transcription system that she developed
and that is used by conversation analysts world-wide. Her research
papers were a distinctive and original voice in the emerging
micro-analysis of interaction in everyday life.
The Shape of Thought: How Mental Adaptations Evolve presents a road
map for an evolutionary psychology of the twenty-first century. It
brings together theory from biology and cognitive science to show
how the brain can be composed of specialized adaptations, and yet
also an organ of plasticity. Although mental adaptations have
typically been seen as monolithic, hard-wired components frozen in
the evolutionary past, The Shape of Thought presents a new view of
mental adaptations as diverse and variable, with distinct functions
and evolutionary histories that shape how they develop, what
information they use, and what they do with that information. The
book describes how advances in evolutionary developmental biology
can be applied to the brain by focusing on the design of the
developmental systems that build it. Crucially, developmental
systems can be plastic, designed by the process of natural
selection to build adaptive phenotypes using the rich information
available in our social and physical environments. This approach
bridges the long-standing divide between "nativist" approaches to
development, based on innateness, and "empiricist" approaches,
based on learning. It shows how a view of humans as a flexible,
culturally-dependent species is compatible with a complexly
specialized brain, and how the nature of our flexibility can be
better understood by confronting the evolved design of the organ on
which that flexibility depends.
How does the immediate experience of musical sound relate to
processes of meaning construction and discursive mediation?
This question lies at the heart of the studies presented in
Experience and Meaning in Music Performance, a unique
multi-authored work that both draws on and contributes to current
debates in a wide range of disciplines, including ethnomusicology,
musicology, psychology, and cognitive science. Addressing a wide
range of musical practices from Indian raga and Afro-Brazilian
Congado rituals to jazz, rock, and Canadian aboriginal fiddling,
the coherence of this study is underpinned by its three main
themes: experience, meaning, and performance. Central to all of the
studies are moments of performance: those junctures when sound and
meaning are actually produced. Experience-what people do, and what
they feel, while engaging in music-is equally important. And
considered alongside these is meaning: what people put into a
performance, what they (and others) get out of it, and, more
broadly, how discourses shape performances and experiences of
music. In tracing trajectories from moments of musical execution,
this volume a novel and productive view of how cultural practice
relates to the experience and meaning of musical performance.
A model of interdisciplinary study, and including access to an
array of audio-visual materials available on an extensive companion
website, Experience and Meaning in Music Performance is essential
reading for scholars and students of ethnomusicology and music
psychology.
Educators' most important work is to help students develop the
intellectual and social strength of character necessary to live
well in the world. The way to do this, argue authors Bena Kallick
and Allison Zmuda, is to increase the say students have in their
own learning and prepare them to navigate complexities they face
both inside and beyond school. This means rethinking traditional
teacher and student roles and re-examining goal setting, lesson
planning, assessment, and feedback practices. It means establishing
classrooms that prioritize: Voice-Involving students in "the what"
and "the how" of learning and equipping them to be stewards of
their own education. Co-creation-Guiding students to identify the
challenges and concepts they want to explore and outline the
actions they will take. Social construction-Having students work
with others to theorize, pursue common goals, build products, and
generate performances. Self-discovery-Teaching students to reflect
on their own developing skills and knowledge so that they will
acquire new understandings of themselves and how they learn. Based
on their exciting work in the field, Kallick and Zmuda map out a
transformative model of personalization that puts students at the
center and asks them to employ the set of dispositions for
engagement and learning known as the Habits of Mind. They share the
perspectives of educators engaged in this work; highlight the
habits that empower students to pursue aspirations, investigate
problems, design solutions, chase curiosities, and create
performances; and provide tools and recommendations for adjusting
classroom practices to facilitate learning that is self-directed,
dynamic, sometimes messy, and always meaningful.
The study of attention is central to psychology. In this work,
Michael Posner, a pioneer in attention research, presents the
science of attention in a larger social context, which includes our
ability to voluntarily choose and act upon an object of thought.
The volume is based on fifty years of research involving
behavioral, imaging, developmental, and genetic methods. It
describes three brain networks of attention that carry out the
functions of obtaining and maintaining the alert state, orienting
to sensory events, and regulating responses. The book ties these
brain networks to anatomy, connectivity, development, and
socialization and includes material on pathologies that involve
attentional networks, as well as their role in education and social
interaction.
It is well known that the class of steroid hormones known as
estrogens have powerful effects on organs related to reproduction
such as the uterus and the breast. What is less well known is that
estrogens also profoundly modulate brain function and behavior.
Estrogens, such as estradiol, can occur in brain as the result of
ovarian secretion of the hormone into the blood that then finds its
way to the brain. In male vertebrates, the testes secrete
androgens, such as testosterone, into the blood and this class of
steroid hormones can be converted into estrogens in the brain via
the action of the enzyme aromatase which is expressed in the male
brain in many species. Finally estradiol can be synthesized de novo
from cholesterol as it has been shown in a variety of species that
all the enzymes required to synthesize estrogens are expressed in
the brain. This book collects chapters by experts in the field that
considers, how estradiol is synthesized in the brain and what its
effects are on a variety of behaviors. Special attention is paid to
the enzyme aromatase that is distributed in discrete regions of the
brain and is highly regulated in a sex specific and seasonal
specific manner. Recently it has become clear that estrogens can
act in the brain at two very different time scales, one is rather
long lasting (days to weeks) and involves the modulation of gene
transcription by the hormone-receptor complex. A second mode of
action is much quicker and involves the action of estrogens on cell
membranes that can result in effects on second messenger systems
and ultimately behavior within minutes. Thus this book highlights
novel views of estrogen action that are still under-appreciated
namely that estrogens have significant effects on the male brain
and that they can act on two very different times scales. This
volume will be of interest to both basic researchers and clinicians
interested in the action of estrogens.
While the fall of the Berlin Wall is positively commemorated in the
West, the intervening years have shown that the former Soviet Bloc
has a more complicated view of its legacy. In post-communist
Eastern Europe, the way people remember state socialism is closely
intertwined with the manner in which they envision historical
justice. Twenty Years After Communism is concerned with the
explosion of a politics of memory triggered by the fall of state
socialism in Eastern Europe, and it takes a comparative look at the
ways that communism and its demise have been commemorated (or not
commemorated) by major political actors across the region. The book
is built on three premises. The first is that political actors
always strive to come to terms with the history of their
communities in order to generate a sense of order in their personal
and collective lives. Second, new leaders sometimes find it
advantageous to mete out justice on the politicians of abolished
regimes, and whether and how they do so depends heavily on their
interpretation and assessment of the collective past. Finally,
remembering the past, particularly collectively, is always a
political process, thus the politics of memory and commemoration
needs to be studied as an integral part of the establishment of new
collective identities and new principles of political legitimacy.
Each chapter takes a detailed look at the commemorative ceremony of
a different country of the former Soviet Bloc. Collectively the
book looks at patterns of extrication from state socialism,
patterns of ethnic and class conflict, the strategies of communist
successor parties, and the cultural traditions of a given country
that influence the way official collective memory is constructed.
Twenty Years After Communism develops a new analytical and
explanatory framework that helps readers to understand the utility
of historical memory as an important and understudied part of
democratization.
Largely through trial and error, filmmakers have developed engaging
techniques that capture our sensations, thoughts, and feelings.
Philosophers and film theorists have thought deeply about the
nature and impact of these techniques, yet few scientists have
delved into empirical analyses of our movie experience-or what
Arthur P. Shimamura has coined "psychocinematics." This edited
volume introduces this exciting field by bringing together film
theorists, philosophers, psychologists, and neuroscientists to
consider the viability of a scientific approach to our movie
experience.
Adult cognitive development is one of the most important yet most
neglected aspects in the study of human psychology. Although the
development of cognition and intelligence during childhood and
adolescence is of great interest to researchers, educators, and
parents, many assume that this development stops progressing in any
significant manner when people reach adulthood. In fact, cognition
and intelligence do continue to progress in very significant ways.
In this second edition of Developmental Influences on Adult
Intelligence, K. Warner Schaie presents the history, latest data,
and results from the Seattle Longitudinal Study (SLS). The purpose
of the SLS is to study various aspects of psychological development
during the adult years. Initiated in 1956 and focusing on a random
sample of 500 adults ranging in age from 25 to 95 years old, the
SLS is organized around five questions: Does intelligence change
uniformly throughout adulthood, or are there different
life-course-ability patterns? At what age and at what magnitude can
decrement in ability be reliably detected? What are the patterns
and magnitude of generational differences? What accounts for
individual differences in age-related change in adulthood? Can the
intellectual decline that increases with age be reversed by
educational intervention? The first edition of the book provided an
account of the SLS through the 1998 (seventh wave) data collection
and of the associated family study through the 1996 (second wave)
data collection. Since that time, Schaie and his collaborators have
conducted several additional data collections. These include a
further longitudinal follow-up in 2005/06, a longitudinal follow-up
and 3rd data collection for the family study in 2003/04, and
acquisition of a 3rd generation sample in 2002. Hence, virtually
all of the content from the first edition has been updated and
expanded, and three new chapters are included on Health Behaviors
and Intellectual Functioning, Biological Influences on Cognitive
Change, and Prediction of Individual Cognitive Decline. This new
edition is a valuable resource for researchers and practitioners
specializing in adult development, aging, and adult education, as
well as students and faculty in developmental, cognitive, and
social psychology, psychiatry, nursing, social work, and the social
sciences interested in issues of human aging.
Eye movements are a vital part of our interaction with the world.
They play a pivotal role in perception, cognition, and education.
Research in this field is now proceeding at a considerable pace and
casting new light on how the eyes move and what information we can
derive during the frequent and brief periods of fixation. However,
the origins of this work are less well known, even though much of
our knowledge was derived from this research with far more
primitive equipment. This book is unique in tracing the history of
eye movement research. It shows how great strides were made in this
area before modern recording devices were available, especially in
the measurement of nystagmus. When photographic techniques were
adapted to measure discontinuous eye movements, from about 1900,
many of the issues that are now basic to modern research were then
investigated. One of the earliest cognitive tasks examined was
reading, and it remains in the vanguard of contemporary research.
Modern researchers in this field will be astonished at the
subtleties of these early experimental studies and the ingenuity of
interpretations that were advanced one and even two centuries ago.
Though physicians often carried out the original eye movement
research, later on it was pursued by psychologists - it is within
contemporary neuroscience that we find these two strands reunited.
Anyone interested in the origins of psychology and neuroscience
will find much to stimulate and surprise them in this valuable new
work.
What is consciousness? How does the subjective character of
consciousness fit into an objective world? How can there be a
science of consciousness? In this sequel to his groundbreaking and
controversial The Conscious Mind, David Chalmers develops a unified
framework that addresses these questions and many others. Starting
with a statement of the "hard problem" of consciousness, Chalmers
builds a positive framework for the science of consciousness and a
nonreductive vision of the metaphysics of consciousness. He replies
to many critics of The Conscious Mind, and then develops a positive
theory in new directions. The book includes original accounts of
how we think and know about consciousness, of the unity of
consciousness, and of how consciousness relates to the external
world. Along the way, Chalmers develops many provocative ideas: the
"consciousness meter", the Garden of Eden as a model of perceptual
experience, and The Matrix as a guide to the deepest philosophical
problems about consciousness and the external world. This book will
be required reading for anyone interested in the problems of mind,
brain, consciousness, and reality.
Decision-making is an activity in which everyone is engaged on a
more or less daily basis. In this book, Karin Brunsson and Nils
Brunsson explore the intricacies of decision-making for individuals
and organizations. When, how and why do they make decisions? The
authors identify four distinct ways of reasoning that
decision-makers use. The consequences of decisions vary: some
promote action, others impede it, and some produce more
responsibility than others. With in-depth discussions of
rationality, justifications and hypocrisy, the authors show how
organizational and political decision processes become
over-complicated and difficult for both decision makers and
external observers to understand. Decisions is a concise and
easy-to-read introduction to a highly significant and intriguing
topic. Based on research from several fields, it provides useful
reading and essential knowledge for scholars and students
throughout the social sciences and for everyone who wants to
understand their own decisions and those of others.
|
|