|
Books > Language & Literature > Language & linguistics > Semantics (meaning)
Ernest Lepore and Kirk Ludwig examine the foundations and
applications of Davidson's influential program of truth-theoretic
semantics for natural languages. The program uses an axiomatic
truth theory for a language, which meets certain constraints, to
serve the goals of a compositional meaning theory. Lepore and
Ludwig explain and clarify the motivations for the approach, and
then consider how to apply the framework to a range of important
natural language constructions, including quantifiers, proper
names, indexicals, simple and complex demonstratives, quotation,
adjectives and adverbs, the simple and perfect tenses, temporal
adverbials and temporal quantifiers, tense in sentential complement
clauses, attitude and indirect discourse reports, and the problem
of interrogative and imperative sentences. They not only discuss
Davidson's own contributions to these subjects but consider
criticisms, developments, and alternatives as well. They conclude
with a discussion of logical form in natural language in light of
the approach, the role of the concept of truth in the program, and
Davidson's view of it. Anyone working on meaning will find this
book invaluable.
Abstract objects have been a central topic in philosophy since
antiquity. Philosophers have defended various views about abstract
objects by appealing to metaphysical considerations, considerations
regarding mathematics or science, and, not infrequently, intuitions
about natural language. This book pursues the question of how and
whether natural language allows for reference to abstract objects
in a fully systematic way. By making full use of contemporary
linguistic semantics, it presents a much greater range of
linguistic generalizations than has previously been taken into
consideration in philosophical discussions, and it argues for an
ontological picture is very different from that generally taken for
granted by philosophers and semanticists alike. Reference to
abstract objects such as properties, numbers, propositions, and
degrees is considerably more marginal than generally held. Instead,
natural language is rather generous in allowing reference to
particularized properties (tropes), the use of nonreferential
expressions in apparent referential position, and the use of
"nominalizing expressions," such as quantifiers like "something."
Reference to abstract objects is achieved generally only by the use
of 'reifying terms', such as "the number eight."
How Words Mean introduces a new approach to the role of words and
other linguistic units in the construction of meaning. It does so
by addressing the interaction between non-linguistic concepts and
the meanings encoded in language. It develops an account of how
words are understood when we produce and hear language in situated
contexts of use. It proposes two theoretical constructs, the
lexical concept and the cognitive model. These are central to the
accounts of lexical representation and meaning construction
developed, giving rise to the Theory of Lexical Concepts and
Cognitive Models (or LCCM Theory).
Vyvyan Evans integrates and advances recent developments in
cognitive science, particularly in cognitive linguistics and
cognitive psychology. He builds a framework for the understanding
and analysis of meaning that is at once descriptively adequate and
psychologically plausible. In so doing he also addresses current
issues in lexical semantics and semantic compositionality,
polysemy, figurative language, and the semantics of time and space,
and writes in a way that will be accessible to students of
linguistics and cognitive science at advanced undergraduate level
and above.
David Charles presents a study of Aristotle's views on meaning, essence, necessity, and related topics. These interconnected views are central to Aristotle's metaphysics, philosophy of language, and philosophy of science. They are also highly relevant to current debates in philosophy of language. Charles aims, on the basis of a careful reading of Aristotle's texts and many subsequent works, to reach a clear understanding of his claims and arguments, and to assess their truth and their importance to philosophy ancient and modern.
The phenomenon of unaccusativity is a central focus for the study
of the complex properties of verb classes. The Unaccusative
Hypothesis, first formulated in 1978, claimed that there are two
classes of intransitive verbs, the unaccusative (Jill arrived) and
the unergative or agentive (Jill sings). The hypothesis has
provided a rich context for debating whether syntactic behaviour is
semantically or lexically determined, the consequence of syntactic
context, or a combination of these factors. No consensus has been
reached. This book combines contemporary approaches to the subject
with several papers that have achieved a significant status even
though formally unpublished.
Relativism has dominated many intellectual circles, past and
present, but the twentieth century saw it banished to the fringes
of mainstream analytic philosophy. Of late, however, it is making
something of a comeback within that loosely configured tradition, a
comeback that attempts to capitalize on some important ideas in
foundational semantics. Relativism and Monadic Truth aims not
merely to combat analytic relativism but also to combat the
foundational ideas in semantics that led to its revival. Doing so
requires a proper understanding of the significance of possible
worlds semantics, an examination of the relation between truth and
the flow of time, an account of putatively relevant data from
attitude and speech act reporting, and a careful treatment of
various operators. Throughout, Herman Cappelen and John Hawthorne
contrast relativism with a view according to which the contents of
thought and talk are propositions that instantiate the fundamental
monadic properties of truth simpliciter and falsity simpliciter.
Such propositions, they argue, are the semantic values of sentences
(relative to context), the objects of illocutionary acts, and,
unsurprisingly, the objects of propositional attitudes.
This book investigates the nature of generalization in language and
examines how language is known by adults and acquired by children.
It looks at how and why constructions are learned, the relation
between their forms and functions, and how cross-linguistic and
language-internal
generalizations about them can be explained.
Constructions at Work is divided into three parts: in the first
Professor Goldberg provides an overview of constructionist
approaches, including the constructionist approach to argument
structure, and argues for a usage-based model of grammar. In Part
II she addresses issues concerning how
generalizations are constrained and constructional generalizations
are learned. In Part III the author shows that a combination of
function and processing accounts for a wide range of
language-internal and cross-linguistic generalizations. She then
considers the degree to which the function of
constructions explains their distribution and examines
cross-linguistic tendencies in argument realization. She
demonstrates that pragmatic and cognitive processes account for the
data without appeal to stipulations that are language-specific.
This book is an important contribution to the study of how language
operates in the mind and in the world and how these operations
relate. It is of central interest for scholars and graduate-level
students in all branches of theoretical linguistics and
psycholinguistics. It will also appeal to
cognitive scientists and philosophers concerned with language and
its acquisition.
Contact Linguistics is a critical investigation of what happens to
the grammars of languages when bilingual speakers use both their
languages in the same clause. It consolidates earlier insights and
presents the new theoretical and empirical work of a scholar whose
ideas have had a fundamental impact on the field. It also shows
that bilingual data offer a revealing window on the structure of
the language faculty. Carol Myers-Scotton examines the nature of
major contact phenomena, especially lexical borrowing, grammatical
convergence, codeswitching, first language attrition, mixed
languages, and the development of creoles. She argues forcefully
that types of contact phenomena often seen as separate in fact
result from the same processes and can be explained by the same
principles. Her discussion centers around two new models derived
from the Matrix Language Frame model, previously applied only to
codeswitching. One model recognizes four types of morphemes based
on their different patterns of distribution across contact
phenomena; its key hyothesis is that distribution depends on
differential access to the morphemes in the production process. The
other analyzes three levels of abstract lexical structure whose
splitting and recombination across languages in bilingual speech
explains many contact outcomes. This is an important volume, of
unusual relevance for theories of competence and performance and
vital for all those concerned with language contact. Carol
Myers-Scotton is a Carolina Distinguished Professor of Linguistics
at the University of South Carolina. She is a specialist in
language contact phenomena and sociolinguistics and has a special
interest in East and Southern African linguistics. In 1993, she
published two volumes on codeswitching, Social Motivations for
Codeswitching: Evidence from Africa, and Duelling Languages:
Grammatical Structure in Codeswitching (both OUP). She has also
edited a volume of essays on language and literature (OUP 1998) and
published many articles in her areas of interest.
 |
Meaning
(Hardcover)
Stephen Schiffer
|
R2,590
Discovery Miles 25 900
|
Ships in 12 - 17 working days
|
|
What is it for marks or sounds to have meaning, and what is it for
someone to mean something in producing them? Answering these and
related questions, Schiffer explores communication, speech acts,
convention, and the meaning of linguistic items in this reissue of
a seminal work on the foundations of meaning. A new introduction
takes account of recent developments and places his theory in a
broader context.
Semantic alignment refers to a type of language that has two means
of morphosyntactically encoding the arguments of intransitive
predicates, typically treating these as an agent or as a patient of
a transitive predicate, or else by a means of a treatment that
varies according to lexical aspect. This collection of new
typological and case studies is the first book-length investigation
of semantically aligned languages for three decades. Leading
international typologists explore the differences and commonalities
of languages with semantic alignment systems and compare the
structure of these languages to languages without them. They look
at how such systems arise or disappear and provide areal overviews
of Eurasia, the Americas, and the south-west Pacific, the areas
where semantically aligned languages are concentrated. This book
will interest typological and historical linguists at graduate
level and above.
A truly original book in every sense of the word, The Dictionary of
Obscure Sorrows poetically defines emotions that we all feel but
don't have the words to express, until now-from the creator of the
popular online project of the same name. Have you ever wondered
about the lives of each person you pass on the street, realizing
that everyone is the main character in their own story, each living
a life as vivid and complex as your own? That feeling has a name:
"sonder." Or maybe you've watched a thunderstorm roll in and felt a
primal hunger for disaster, hoping it would shake up your life.
That's called "lachesism." Or you were looking through old photos
and felt a pang of nostalgia for a time you've never actually
experienced. That's "anemoia." If you've never heard of these terms
before, that's because they didn't exist until John Koenig began
his epic quest to fill the gaps in the language of emotion. Born as
a website in 2009, The Dictionary of Obscure Sorrows has garnered
widespread critical acclaim, inspired TED talks, album titles,
cocktails, and even tattoos. The Dictionary of Obscure Sorrows
"creates beautiful new words that we need but do not yet have,"
says John Green, bestselling author of The Fault in Our Stars. By
turns poignant, funny, and mind-bending, the definitions include
whimsical etymologies drawn from languages around the world,
interspersed with otherworldly collages and lyrical essays that
explore forgotten corners of the human condition-from "astrophe,"
the longing to explore beyond the planet Earth, to "zenosyne," the
sense that time keeps getting faster. The Dictionary of Obscure
Sorrows is for anyone who enjoys a shift in perspective, pondering
the ineffable feelings that make up our lives, which have far more
in common than we think. With a gorgeous package and beautifully
illustrated throughout, this is the perfect gift for creatives,
word nerds, and people everywhere.
'This is a life-changing book. Read it three times and then give a copy
to anyone you care about. It will make things better' – Seth Godin,
author of This is Marketing
'All you need is Buster Benson. His methods are instantly actionable,
[and] his writing is funny and relatable' – Adam Grant, author of
Originals
Why Are We Yelling is Buster Benson's essential guide to having more
honest and constructive arguments.
The way we argue is broken. Whether it’s about Brexit, the existence of
ghosts, the best burger in the city or who’s allowed to sit in your
favourite chair, we end up digging our heels in and yelling at one
another or choosing to avoid heated topics entirely. There has to be a
better way.
Buster Benson, a Silicon Valley entrepreneur with two decades of
experience facilitating hard conversations at some of the biggest tech
companies in the world, recommends eight things to try in order to make
disagreements more productive. By applying these eight new habits, we
can flip frustrating, unproductive disagreements into ones that bear
fruit and bring people closer together.
In this book you'll master practical skills to make your disagreements
more productive by:
- Understanding four ways of disagreeing that are more valuable than
simply ‘winning’ the argument
- Identifying the kind of argument you’re having so you know how best
to negotiate it
- Articulating the best possible version of your opponent’s argument
before attacking it
With this toolkit we can explore more possibilities and perspectives in
the world, simply because we’ll no longer be afraid to wade into scary
topics of conversation.
|
|