|
Books > Social sciences > Psychology > Philosophy & theory of psychology > General
Our modern-day word for sympathy is derived from the classical
Greek word for fellow-feeling. Both in the vernacular as well as in
the various specialist literatures within philosophy, psychology,
neuroscience, economics, and history, "sympathy" and "empathy" are
routinely conflated. In practice, they are also used to refer to a
large variety of complex, all-too-familiar social phenomena: for
example, simultaneous yawning or the giggles. Moreover, sympathy is
invoked to address problems associated with social dislocation and
political conflict. It is, then, turned into a vehicle toward
generating harmony among otherwise isolated individuals and a way
for them to fit into a larger whole, be it society and the
universe. This volume offers a historical overview of some of the
most significant attempts to come to grips with sympathy in Western
thought from Plato to experimental economics. The contributors are
leading scholars in philosophy, classics, history, economics,
comparative literature, and political science. Sympathy is
originally developed in Stoic thought. It was also taken up by
Plotinus and Galen. There are original contributed chapters on each
of these historical moments. Use for the concept was re-discovered
in the Renaissance. And the volume has original chapters not just
on medical and philosophical Renaissance interest in sympathy, but
also on the role of antipathy in Shakespeare and the significance
of sympathy in music theory. Inspired by the influence of Spinoza,
sympathy plays a central role in the great moral psychologies of,
say, Anne Conway, Leibniz, Hume, Adam Smith, and Sophie De Grouchy
during the eighteenth century. The volume should offers an
introduction to key background concept that is often overlooked in
many of the most important philosophies of the early modern period.
About a century ago the idea of Einfuhlung (or empathy) was
developed in theoretical philosophy, then applied in practical
philosophy and the newly emerging scientific disciplines of
psychology. Moreover, recent economists have rediscovered sympathy
in part experimentally and, in part by careful re-reading of the
classics of the field.
Philip Pettit has drawn together here a series of interconnected
essays on three subjects to which he has made notable
contributions. The first part of the book discusses the
rule-following character of thought. The second considers how
choice can be responsive to different sorts of factors, while still
being under the control of thought and the reasons that thought
marshals. The third examines the implications of this view of
choice and rationality for the normative regulation of social
behaviour.
Consciousness is a perennial source of mystification in the
philosophy of mind: how can processes in the brain amount to
conscious experiences? Robert Kirk uses the notion of `raw feeling'
to bridge the intelligibility gap between our knowledge of
ourselves as physical organisms and our knowledge of ourselves as
subjects of experience; he argues that there is no need for
recourse to dualism or private mental objects. The task is to
understand how the truth about raw feeling could be strictly
implied by narrowly physical truths. Kirk's explanation turns on an
account of what it is to be a subject of conscious perceptual
experience. He offers penetrating analyses of the problems of
consciousness and suggests novel solutions which, unlike their
rivals, can be accepted without gritting one's teeth. His sustained
defence of non-reductive physicalism shows that we need not abandon
hope of finding a solution to the mind-body problem.
This book is a philosophical exploration of disorientation and its
significance for action. Disorientations are human experiences of
losing one's bearings, such that life is disrupted and it is not
clear how to go on. In the face of life experiences like trauma,
grief, illness, migration, education, queer identification, and
consciousness raising, individuals can be deeply disoriented. These
and other disorientations are not rare. Although disorientations
can be common and powerful parts of individuals' lives, they remain
uncharacterized by Western philosophers, and overlooked by
ethicists. Disorientations can paralyze, overwhelm, embitter, and
misdirect moral agents, and moral philosophy and motivational
psychology have important insights to offer into why this is. More
perplexing are the ways disorientations may prompt improved moral
action. Ami Harbin draws on first person accounts, philosophical
texts, and qualitative and quantitative research to show that in
some cases of disorientation, individuals gain new forms of
awareness of political complexity and social norms, and new habits
of relating to others and an unpredictable moral landscape. She
then argues for the moral and political promise of these gains. A
major contention of the book is that disorientations have
'non-resolutionary effects': they can help us act without first
helping us resolve what to do. In exploring these possibilities,
Disorientation and Moral Life contributes to philosophy of
emotions, moral philosophy, and political thought from a distinctly
feminist perspective. It makes the case for seeing disorientations
as having the power to motivate profound and long-term shifts in
moral and political action. A feminist re-envisioning of moral
psychology provides the framework for understanding how they do so.
Cognitive Systems and the Extended Mind surveys philosophical
issues raised by the situated movement in cognitive science, that
is, the treatment of cognitive phenomena as the joint products of
brain, body, and environment. The book focuses primarily on the
hypothesis of extended cognition, which asserts that human
cognitive processes literally comprise elements beyond the boundary
of the human organism. Rupert argues that the only plausible way in
which to demarcate cognitions is systems-based: cognitive states or
processes are the states of the integrated set of mechanisms and
capacities that contribute causally and distinctively to the
production of cognitive phenomena--for example, language-use,
memory, decision-making, theory construction, and, more
importantly, the associated forms of behavior. Rupert argues that
this integrated system is most likely to appear within the
boundaries of the human organism. He argues that the systems-based
view explains the existing successes of cognitive psychology and
cognate fields in a way that extended conceptions of cognition do
not, and that once the systems-based view has been adopted, it is
especially clear how extant arguments in support of the extended
view go wrong.
Cognitive Systems and the Extended Mind also examines further
aspects of the situated program in cognitive science, including the
embedded and embodied approaches to cognition. Rupert asks to what
extent the plausible incarnations of these situated views depart
from orthodox, computational cognitive science. Here, Rupert
focuses on the notions of representation and computation, arguing
that the embedded and embodied views do not constitute the radical
shifts in perspective they are often claimed to be. Rupert also
argues that, properly understood, the embodied view does not offer
a new role for the body, different in principle from the one
presupposed by orthodox cognitive science.
"Rupert's book is a good read. It is a sustained, systematic,
critical examination of the idea that minds are not simply
ensconced inside heads, but extend into both bodies and the world
beyond the body.... There is much to admire in this book. It is
well-structured and well-written, adopting a self-consciously
naturalistic perspective on how to understand the mind -- through
our best, even if imperfect, empirical sciences in the domain of
cognition. By presenting and critiquing a number of explicit
arguments for and against the specific views that Rupert considers,
Cognitive Systems advances the field."-- Notre Dame Philosophical
Reviews
"Rupert's treatment is a state of the art sustained attack on
various forms of the 'extended mind hypothesis'. It is rigorous and
challenging, and will be of interest to a quite a large audience of
researchers (graduates and above) in philosophy and in cognitive
science. Rupert studiously avoids the 'straw men' that populate
some recent critiques, and raises deep and sympathetic challenges
that go to the core of the program."
--Andy Clark, Department of Philosophy, University of Edinburgh
This title, now in its second edition, is an introduction to the
psychological system known as transactional analysis (TA). It is
aimed at the general reader as well as at TA trainees and
practitioners.
Elgar Advanced Introductions are stimulating and thoughtful
introductions to major fields in the social sciences, business and
law, expertly written by the world's leading scholars. Designed to
be accessible yet rigorous, they offer concise and lucid surveys of
the substantive and policy issues associated with discrete subject
areas. Leading scholar Tom R. Tyler provides a timely and engaging
introduction to the field of law and psychology. This Advanced
Introduction outlines the main areas of research, their relevance
to law and the way that psychological findings have shaped - or
failed to shape - the corresponding areas of law. Key features
include: broad coverage of the key topics in the field accessible,
non-technical presentation of research findings focus on the
relevance of psychological theories to topics in law emphasis on
the institutional realities within which law functions discussion
of the problems of bringing research findings into the legal
system. Presenting an informative overview of this rapidly
developing area, the Advanced Introduction to Law and Psychology
will be a key resource for students and scholars of law, psychology
and the social sciences. It will also be of benefit to
psychologists and legal practitioners.
From the bestselling author of This Is Your Brain on Music and The Organized Mind, a surprising and inspiring exploration of the healing power of music.
We are only just beginning to appreciate the healing power of music. In recent years, a wave of scientific research has upended everything we once knew about its effects on our brains: not only in reducing stress, but also in enhancing cognitive function, slowing the spread of neurodegenerative diseases like Parkinson’s and Alzheimer’s, even strengthening our immune systems.
Here, a neuroscientist and celebrated musician introduces a bold new paradigm for medical treatment, rooted in the unexpected influence of music on our minds and bodies. From explaining how ‘rhythmic auditory stimulation’ can fight multiple sclerosis, to examining why Tracy Chapman’s songs might just help cure PTSD, Professor Daniel Levitin offers surprising insights into the new science of music as medicine.
Along the way, he explores how each of us can use music to calm our thoughts, repair our memories and heal our deepest psychological wounds. The result is both a surprising tour through the science of music, and a joyful celebration of humanity’s oldest obsession.
Beneath the surface we are all connected . . . 'An authentically
soothing, powerful, thought-provoker.' MATT HAIG 'On Connection is
medicine for these wounded times.' MAX PORTER 'On Connection came
to me when I needed it most, and reminded me that the links we have
to places, people, words, ourselves, are what keep us alive.'
CANDICE CARTY-WILLIAMS This is a book about connection. About how
immersing ourselves in creativity can help us cultivate greater
self-awareness and bring us closer to each other. Drawing on two
decades of experience as a writer and performer, Kae Tempest
champions the role of creativity - in whatever form we choose to
practice it - as an act of love, helping us establish a deeper
relationship to our true selves, and to others and the world we
live in. Honest, hopeful and written with piercing clarity, On
Connection is an inspiring personal meditation that will transform
the way you see the world. 'Persuasive and profound.' OBSERVER
'Tempest's prose is crisp and thoughtful.' NEW STATESMAN
Over the last several decades, videotestimony with aging Holocaust
survivors has brought these witnesses into the limelight. Yet the
success of these projects has made it seem that little survivor
testimony took place in earlier years. In truth, thousands of
survivors began to recount their experience at the earliest
opportunity. This book provides the first full-length case study of
early postwar Holocaust testimony, focusing on David Boder's 1946
displaced persons interview project. In July 1946, Boder, a
psychologist, traveled to Europe to interview victims of the
Holocaust who were in the Displaced Persons (DP) camps and what he
called "shelter houses." During his nine weeks in Europe, Boder
carried out approximately 130 interviews in nine languages and
recorded them on a state-of-the-art wire recorder. Likely the
earliest audio recorded testimony of Holocaust survivors, the
interviews are today the earliest extant recordings, valuable for
the spoken word (that of the DP narrators and of Boder himself) and
also for the song sessions and religious services that Boder wire
recorded at various points through the expedition. Eighty were
eventually transcribed into English, most of which were included in
a self-published manuscript of more than 3,100 pages. Rosen sets
Boder's project in the context of the postwar response to displaced
persons, sketches the dramatic background of his previous life and
work, chronicles in detail the evolving process of interviewing
both Jewish and non-Jewish DPs, and examines from several angles
the implications for the history of Holocaust testimony. Such
postwar testimony, Rosen avers, deserves to be taken on its own
terms-as unbelated testimony-rather than to be enfolded into
earlier or later schemas of testimony. Moreover, Boder's efforts
and the support he was given for them demonstrate that American
postwar response to the Holocaust was not universally indifferent
but rather often engaged, concerned, and resourceful.
Also available in an open-access, full-text edition athttp:
//txspace.tamu.edu/bitstream/handle/1969.1/88024/Cambray_Synchronicity_9781603441438_txt.pdf?sequence=4
In 1952 C. G. Jung published a paradoxical hypothesis on
synchronicity that marked an attempt to expand the western world's
conception of the relationship between nature and the psyche.
Jung's hypothesis sought to break down the polarizing cause-effect
assessment of the world and psyche, suggesting that everything is
interconnected. Thus, synchronicity is both "a meaningful event"
and "an acausal connecting principle." Evaluating the world in this
manner opened the door to "exploring the possibility of meaning in
chance or random events, deciphering if and when meaning might be
present even if outside conscious awareness."
Now, after contextualizing Jung's work in relation to contemporary
scientific advancements such as relativity and quantum theories,
Joseph Cambray explores in this book how Jung's theories,
practices, and clinical methods influenced the current field of
complexity theory, which works with a paradox similar to Jung's
synchronicity: the importance of symmetry as well as the need to
break that symmetry for "emergence" to occur. Finally, Cambray
provides his unique contribution to the field by attempting to
trace "cultural synchronicities," a reconsideration of historical
events in terms of their synchronistic aspects. For example, he
examines the emergence of democracy in ancient Greece in order "to
find a model of group decision making based on emergentist
principles with a synchronistic core."
![Misfit (Hardcover): Shruti Mishra](//media.loot.co.za/images/x80/1299588759345179215.jpg) |
Misfit
(Hardcover)
Shruti Mishra
|
R697
R594
Discovery Miles 5 940
Save R103 (15%)
|
Ships in 10 - 15 working days
|
|
![Jung (Paperback): Anthony Storr](//media.loot.co.za/images/x80/688294883328179215.jpg) |
Jung
(Paperback)
Anthony Storr
|
R221
Discovery Miles 2 210
|
Ships in 12 - 17 working days
|
|
Carl Gustav Jung first wanted to be an archaeologist. When family
finances made this impossible, he pursued a medical degree from
Basel University. Considering a specialization in surgery, he
stumbled upon a psychiatry book--an event that changed the course
of his life and ultimately the whole field of depth psychology.
Anthony Storr looks at these and other telling facts about Jung's
life in the opening chapter of this highly acclaimed book. Though
fundamentally concerned with Jung's ideas, Storr's approach shows
his conviction that "in the realm of dynamic psychology it is
impossible to separate ideas from the personality of the man in
whom they occurred." His clear and concise review of the whole
corpus of Jung's writings always keeps in sight the man behind the
work, as each subsequent chapter concentrates on a particular
Jungian concept, guiding the reader through the life and ideas of
this great thinker.
Storr investigates the major principles of analytical psychology,
presenting such central concepts as the collective unconscious, the
archetypes, the shadow, and the persona. He looks at Jung's
religious turmoil and his inner need to reconcile the opposition
between objectivity and subjectivity. He shows how his ideas follow
a progression from the intellectual agitation that characterized
the young psychologist to the advanced theories of balance and
integration found in the mature man. Storr concludes his book with
a look at psychotherapy, describing advances as well as problems
involved in a practical consideration of Jungian techniques.
Perhaps the most remarkable element of "Jung" is its illumination
of complex concepts--concepts that had they been easily accessible
in the original would have caused a wider appreciation of Jung's
work. The clarity and order that Storr brings to light in Jung's
psychology will come as a welcome surprise to those who have found
him an obscure if provocative thinker. Storr's sensitive analysis
makes the book compelling reading for everyone interested in Jung,
and his clear exposition provides a superior introduction for
newcomers, allowing the genius of Jung to appear for the widest
possible audience.
First published in 1973, this classic study is now available again.
The Routledge edition includes a brief preface in which the author
describes his previous work on Jung as well as his meeting with the
great Swiss thinker.
|
You may like...
Behold
Lauren Sleeman
Hardcover
R860
Discovery Miles 8 600
|